U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

Harris R, Sims S, Leamy M, et al. Intentional rounding in hospital wards to improve regular interaction and engagement between nurses and patients: a realist evaluation. Southampton (UK): NIHR Journals Library; 2019 Oct. (Health Services and Delivery Research, No. 7.35.)

Cover of Intentional rounding in hospital wards to improve regular interaction and engagement between nurses and patients: a realist evaluation

Intentional rounding in hospital wards to improve regular interaction and engagement between nurses and patients: a realist evaluation.

Show details

Chapter 8Realist evaluation of intentional rounding: data synthesis

This chapter addresses objectives 6 and 8, reporting the key findings from the realist evaluation to examine the barriers to and facilitators of the successful implementation of IR and synthesising the data from each phase of the study to identify what aspects of IR work, for whom, in what circumstances and why. As part of the synthesis process, a second stakeholder consultation event took place, where attendees considered the findings and how they fitted with their own knowledge and experience of IR. The data reported here includes interview and observation data from the six wards in the three case study sites.

Aims and objectives

The study aims to understand how IR ‘works’ in England, for whom and in what circumstances; to suggest ways to improve its effectiveness; to inform decisions about its implementation in other contexts; and to understand what was causing variations in implementation or outcomes.

Findings

This section is divided into four parts. First, the initial and revised, evidence-informed programme theory is presented to give a broad overview of how IR ‘works’. Second, more detail is provided by outlining the four layers of contextual factors that enable or inhibit the activation of mechanisms. Third, implementation fidelity in England and adaption of the US version of IR (i.e. the Studer Group’s protocol) is examined, followed by a final description of detailed CMO configurations and supporting data.

Programme theory

All programmes (henceforth called ‘interventions’) will implicitly or explicitly have a programme theory or theories68 about how the intervention is expected to cause its intended outcomes. When an intervention such as IR is implemented, it is testing a theory about what ‘might cause change’, even though that theory may not be explicit.69 As previously explained, in the first phase of the research, the initial theories of IR, or mechanisms by which change might occur, were not very explicit (see Chapter 3). Thus, a key task in the realist synthesis was to make the theories in the intervention explicit, by developing clear hypotheses about how and for whom IR might ‘work’ and why. Data used to develop the initial programme theory included analysis of academic peer-reviewed articles, policy documents and grey literature, and discussion in an initial stakeholder consultation event involving service user representatives and NHS clinical staff. These approaches enabled the development of initial programme theories about how, for whom and under what circumstances IR helps nurses with their work (Box 4).

Box Icon

BOX 4

Initial programme theory

Revised evidence-informed programme theory

The revised evidence-informed programme theory is presented in Box 5. An iterative CMO configuration process was used, distilling from the CMOs how IR (the intervention) causes its intended outcomes, based on an analysis of the data. In particular, the degree of support for each CMO configuration was examined within different data sources (e.g. nurses, managers, patients, carers, other health-care professionals) and obtained by different data collection methods (e.g. interviews, observations, national survey).

Box Icon

BOX 5

Revised evidence-informed programme theory

There was some evidence to suggest that the ‘consistency and comprehensiveness’ and ‘accountability’ mechanisms were at least partially activated, in some contexts. Both CMOs were considerably more complex than the initial programme theories and CMOs had implied. Some nurses said IR helped them ‘communicate with their colleagues and within teams’, but this was not supported through the observations or any other source. When activated, the outcomes that were most closely linked to these mechanisms were as follows:

  • All nurses knew what was required to deliver basic, fundamental patient care to a minimum standard.
  • Nurses said that they could use IR documentation to provide evidence that they had delivered basic, fundamental patient care to a minimum standard.
  • Nurses said that they thought that IR led to improvements in levels of patient comfort and safety.

The evidence was inconclusive around whether or not the ‘anticipation’ and ‘visibility and presence’ mechanisms were activated, because, in practice, it was often hard or impossible to separate IR from nurses delivering usual care. Finally, there was no evidence to support the activation of the ’nurse–patient relationships’, ‘allocated time to care’ or ‘patient empowerment’ mechanisms. No additional CMOs were identified during the national survey of senior nursing managers or in the case study fieldwork. The reasons for these findings will be explored in the remainder of this chapter.

Contextual factors influencing variation in implementation and outcomes of intentional rounding

In this section, four layers of context that activate mechanisms and lead to outcomes of IR are examined. Pawson70 and Pawson et al.71 identified four contextual layers that influence variation in outcomes:

  1. individual capabilities and characteristics of key actors
  2. interpersonal relationships
  3. organisational setting
  4. intrastructural setting

This section builds on and refines the contextual factors previously identified in the realist synthesis, based on the case study fieldwork. In Chapters 6 and 7 the perspectives of patients, carers, front-line nurses, senior management and other health-care professionals on organising, delivering and receiving IR were presented. Here, these perspectives are synthesised to describe the enabling and inhibiting factors that determined whether or not mechanisms were activated, operating at each contextual layer. These contextual factors are based on data presented in preceding chapters and/or have been theorised on the basis of these data. For example, the patients and carers whom we interviewed had not received an information leaflet on admission to the wards (as recommended in some of the individual trust IR policy documents from the case studies), but if they had been, we would have inferred that it was more probable that they would have had an awareness of IR.

Individual capabilities and characteristics of key actors

Type of patients
  • Enabling: IR was most beneficial when patients –
    • were elderly and/or vulnerable (e.g. dementia, at risk of falls, wandering, need turning)
    • had difficulties verbally communicating.
  • Inhibiting: IR was least beneficial –
    • when IR was carried out rigidly and not adapted to suit patient needs (see Intentional rounding adaptations)
    • for patients who were relatively well, independent or waiting to go home.
Patient and carer awareness, understanding and involvement
  • Enabling: patients and carers were aware of IR because –
    • they had received a leaflet about IR on admission
    • they were given a verbal explanation about what it was and why it was being done
    • they had engaged in conversations that were prompted by the IR process
    • they had been told when nurses would be coming back to see them as part of IR
    • IR documentation could be easily accessed by patients and their carers (e.g. kept at the end of the bed, as paper copies), so patients and carers had the opportunity and felt able to see what was being recorded, if they wanted to.
  • Inhibiting: patients and carers were not aware of IR. This ‘invisibility’ of IR may have been because –
    • they did not know about IR because they had not been given a written or verbal explanation of it
    • patients were too unwell to comprehend or remember explanations or information about IR
    • they could not distinguish between IR and usual care, because nurses did not ask questions while completing IR (e.g. if they were silently working through a mental IR checklist, or it was done at same time as another nursing task)
    • the IR documentation was not physically accessible for patients and carers to inspect, and/or is considered ‘out of bounds’ to patients and carers.
Nursing staff characteristics
  • Enabling: setting out minimum standards of care for IR could be of most use for nurses who –
    • are unfamiliar with a ward and/or a type of patient (e.g. new or temporary nurses)
    • have less training and/or experience (e.g. HCAs, student nurses)
    • are not performing to the expected minimum standard of care.
  • Inhibiting: setting out minimum standards of care for IR could be of least use for –
    • RNs who had developed their own strategies, practices or mental checklists for ensuring patient comfort and safety. Formal IR processes were unnecessary as they were either already meeting or exceeding the minimum standards of care. Put succinctly, they were doing it already.
    • HCAs who were very experienced and closely supervised by RNs.
Leadership characteristics
  • Enabling: when senior nursing managers –
    • could clearly articulate how IR fitted within the trust’s overall vision and philosophy of nursing care
    • were instrumental in embedding IR in wider organisational processes for ensuring that fundamentals of care were given priority (e.g. documentation, IR part of package of measures for minimising harms)
    • encouraged IR uptake and staff ‘buy-in’, through reminders, tips for success, monitoring performance, rounds experts and senior management walkabouts.
  • Inhibiting: when senior nursing managers –
    • did not support the implementation of IR.

Interpersonal relationships and organisational setting

In Chapter 5, the three case studies were described in terms of their ward profiles, safety thermometer data and costing. In the following sections, comparisons are made across organisational settings in relation to implementation, environmental and structural factors.

Implementation factors
  • Staged or simultaneous implementation.
  • Staff engagement and motivation.
  • Staff education, training and understanding.
  • Fidelity and adaptation (see Fidelity to the original intervention).
  • Design and suitability of IR documentation.
Environmental and structural factors
  • Ward setting and layout.
  • Job demands, workload and nurse staffing levels.
  • Skill mix.
Staged or simultaneous implementation approach
  • Enabling: implementation was most effective when –
    • IR was introduced locally via multidisciplinary staff working groups to develop documentation, piloting and staff consultation on revising documentation.
    • There was widespread roll-out and testing in specific settings, wards and with different types of patients.
    • There was an ongoing programme of auditing, reviewing and adapting.
  • Inhibiting: implementation was least effective when –
    • IR was introduced with minimum preparation, multidisciplinary staff involvement or consultation.
    • IR was introduced simultaneously to all wards.
Staff engagement and motivation
  • Enabling: IR was most useful when nurses and other health-care staff saw and appreciated the benefits of IR (e.g. as a prompt to remember specific items to maintain care and minimise harms).
  • Inhibiting: IR was least useful when nurses and other health-care staff did not see or appreciate the purpose and benefits of IR (e.g. when they saw it as a, ‘tick-box exercise’).
Staff education, training and understanding of intentional rounding
  • Enabling: nurses and other health-care staff had a high level of IR knowledge and understanding of IR. This may be for a number of reasons –
    • IR awareness was embedded in staff inductions.
    • There was clarity in IR instructions and guidance (e.g. for dealing with certain scenarios).
    • There was staff training.
    • Role models were on the ward who could demonstrate and reinforce how to do IR for specific patients.
    • The benefits of IR processes and documentation were obvious to all (e.g. the ward manager gave specific feedback and encouragement, IR was regularly referred to by colleagues).
  • Inhibiting: nurses and other health-care staff had limited knowledge and understanding of IR. This may be because –
    • There was no formal training on IR.
    • Staff were not sure how to complete IR in certain circumstances (e.g. the form did not fit all patients or all wards).
    • There was a lack of effective role models for IR.
    • Staff did not see purpose of IR.
Design and suitability of intentional rounding documentation
  • Enhancing: when documentation layout and instructions –
    • were clear to promote accurate completion
    • were clear on how to adapt (e.g. do risk assessments, determine frequency, decide if not applicable)
    • were adapted to suit ward/patients
    • had high face and content validity.
  • Inhibiting: when documentation was not fit for purpose (for further descriptions of IR documentation variability, see Intentional rounding adaptations).
Environmental and structural factors
Ward setting/layout
  • Enabling: IR was most useful where –
    • ward layout included or consisted entirely of single rooms, so there was scope to increase nursing presence and visibility through IR
    • there was a lack of other initiatives/systems to ensure that nurses were visible and could closely monitor patients.
  • Inhibiting: IR was least useful where –
    • The ward layout used modern design with bays (in our case study sites, bays of 6–8 patients), or in Nightingale wards (where patients were in one large room without subdivisions). This was because nurses were already highly visible to patients in such wards. IR had an added benefit of increasing nursing presence only if it involved nurses speaking directly to patients as part of the process (and not merely looking to assess levels of comfort and safety).
    • Other initiatives/systems were in place to ensure that nurses were visible and closely monitored patients (e.g. in one case study site, the hospital with bays had introduced ‘bay-tagging’ to ensure that there was always a nurse present in the bay, to increase vigilance and to prevent patients getting out of bed unaided, and falling).
Job demands and staffing levels
  • Enabling: IR was most useful when –
    • staffing levels and job demands enabled nurses to complete and document IR without continuous interruptions or having to prioritise other duties
    • nurses were able to document IR at the same time, rather than completing forms retrospectively.
  • Inhibiting: IR was least useful when –
    • it was inhibited because nurses faced competing workload priorities caused by busy wards, interruptions, emergencies or having a high number of complex patients
    • job demands and/or staffing levels meant that nurses had to complete IR forms retrospectively.
Skill mix/workforce stability
  • Enabling: IR was most useful when –
    • the workforce was unstable (e.g. new, temporary, high turnover)
    • there was a high proportion of nurses who were inexperienced and/or junior on the ward.
  • Inhibiting: IR was least useful when –
    • the workforce was stable
    • nurses already used their education, skills, knowledge and experience to exceed the minimum standard of fundamental patient care, as set out by IR.
Senior nursing management/IR organisational policies
  • Enhancing: IR was most useful when –
    • guidance from senior nursing management and IR organisational policies on what was expected was clear, consistent and realistically achievable.
  • Inhibiting: IR was least useful when –
    • guidance from senior nursing management and IR organisational policies were incompatible and/or open to different interpretations.

Intrastructural setting

There were a number of other contextual factors influencing the implementation of IR in England, which are discussed throughout this report and only briefly summarised here.

NHS context

The public inquiry1 led by Sir Robert Francis identified the causes and failings in patient care at Mid Staffordshire NHS Foundation Trust. The report, in three volumes, focused on the neglect of patients, poor standards of care and the adequacy of regulatory and supervisory systems. The inquiry also highlighted issues of negative culture, tolerance of poor standards and disengagement from managerial and leadership responsibilities. Local organisations were asked to take action to strengthen the patient voice, improve front-line care and change culture through leadership. Although there were a few early pioneers of IR in England, having implemented it prior to 2011, widespread adoption of IR occurred only between 2011 and 2014.

Health policy

The level of concern about patient care and patient safety among the public, health-care professionals and politicians was such that it was necessary for the UK government to act quickly to convince the public that the failings identified at the Mid Staffordshire NHS Foundation Trust were being taken seriously and that the recommendations in the Francis report1 were being acted on. Following an announcement by the then prime minister, David Cameron, directors of nursing were told explicitly that they should ‘comply or explain’ why they were not implementing IR. In practice, this meant that many directors of nursing in NHS hospitals perceived the implementation of IR to be mandatory. Our national survey of directors of nursing showed that 97% of trusts had implemented IR in some form, and in at least some wards.

NHS culture

Increasingly, the NHS is described as having a ‘blame culture’, characterised by staff reporting a lack of trust, living with a threat of litigation and fear of reprisals for ‘mistakes’ and that organisations are risk-averse and have a tendency to look for individual ‘mistakes’ rather than for environmental-/system-level explanations. The senior nursing manager interviewees in this study explained at length the daily and ever-increasing challenges they faced in supporting the nursing workforce to deliver high-quality patient care and their fear of ‘being the next Mid Staffs’:

. . . everyone was wary and conscious of Mid-Staffs anyway, and also it was one of those things that, I’m sure every trust and every management team and every organisation did not want Mid-Staffs to be happening in their place.

English implementation fidelity and adaption to the US version of intentional rounding

Fidelity to the original intervention

As stated in Observations of intentional rounding (nurse shadowing and non-participant observation of direct patient care), 188 hours of care delivery were observed by four research staff. A total of 240 IRs were observed in this time, delivered by RNs, HCAs, ward sisters, assistant practitioners and trainee assistant practitioners, and both bank and permanent members of staff. Observations were undertaken throughout the day and evening shifts (ranging between 07.00 and 01.15) on both weekdays and weekends. At every round, researchers recorded whether or not each member of staff conducted the core components of IR according to the Studer Group’s protocol. The following examination of fidelity has been conducted to explore how observations of IR delivered in the English case study sites compare to the original IR intervention as promoted by the Studer Group.

In each intentional round, researchers looked for an opening phrase, examples of the ‘4 P’s’ (pain, positioning, personal needs and placement of items), an environmental assessment, a closing phrase and information about when the staff member would return. Researchers noted whether or not each of these components was observed ‘fully’ (as per the Studer Group protocol), ‘partially’ (notable but not as defined as in the Studer Group protocol) or ‘not observed’. To increase inter-rater reliability between observers, we discussed and agreed how to define each of these categories in the research team; the definitions are provided in Appendix 16 for the sake of transparency.

Researchers also recorded details of when staff members completed the IR documentation (this referred to either ‘immediately after the round’; ‘retrospectively’, i.e. at a later time in the day; ‘prospectively’, i.e. before the round took place; or ‘not at all’. The ‘not at all’ category meant that researchers did not see the nursing staff complete the documentation for the duration of the observation session. The researchers can therefore not be sure that documentation was not completed after the observation session had ended, e.g. at the end of the nursing staff shift). Researchers also recorded any observations of RNs checking that IR had been completed by HCAs (i.e. checking IR documentation to see if HCAs had completed it). On occasion, researchers were unable to determine whether or not a component of IR had been delivered. This was usually because IR was being undertaken at the same time as personal care delivery and the patient and staff member were either behind a drawn curtain or in a single room with the door closed, leaving researchers unable to see or hear the care being delivered. When this was the case, the instances have been recorded in the fidelity table as ‘unable to observe’.

As Appendix 16 highlights, there are a number of caveats to these data. Just because researchers did not observe nursing staff asking patients directly about each of the IR components, one cannot assume that they were not internally assessing them and making clinical judgements as to their relevance. Nursing staff may have felt (rightly or wrongly) that they already knew the answer to some of these questions and, therefore, did not need to ask the patients directly. Similarly, nursing staff may have assessed that particular aspects of the IR protocol were not pertinent to some patients and, therefore, they did not vocalise them. Researchers did not ask nursing staff why they behaved as they did after each round, meaning that they could not be sure what nursing staff were thinking as they performed IR. This is a possible methodological limitation in the study, although it is probable that nursing staff would not have had the time or capacity to debrief with researchers after each round. A further point to remember is that, in some cases, nursing staff were delivering IR when patients were either asleep or away from their bed. In these instances, protocols stated that nursing staff should still complete the IR documentation even though they could not ask patients any of the questions. Instead, they were to mark on their documentation either ‘patient asleep’ or ‘off ward’. Similarly, there were occasions when nursing staff were unable to directly ask patients IR questions either because patients could not speak English/had health issues that made them incapable of responding or because patients had expressed a desire not to be disturbed, yet on these occasions nursing staff were still required to complete the IR documentation. These issues all explain part of the reason why the completion of documentation was observed more commonly than the asking of questions. With these caveats in mind, Table 20 demonstrates how regularly nursing staff were observed to ask patients about or, in the case of environmental assessment, undertake each aspect of the IR protocol.

TABLE 20. Observations of IR in practice: fidelity to the original intervention (i.

TABLE 20

Observations of IR in practice: fidelity to the original intervention (i.e. the Studer Group protocol)

Table 20 shows that 240 individual IR interactions were observed over a period of 188 hours: 89 intentional rounds were observed at case study site 1 (during 68 hours of observation), 108 intentional rounds were observed at case study site 2 (during 60 hours of observation) and 43 intentional rounds were observed at case study site 3 (during 60 hours of observation). A number of other (non IR-related) interactions were also observed in this time frame; these are discussed in Chapter 6, Perspective of other health-care professionals, General contexts and outcomes. Although similar numbers of observational hours were undertaken at each site, far fewer IRs were observed at case study site 3 because clear examples of IR were very difficult to identify. Staff were often seen to incorporate aspects of IR into their general interactions with patients but not to refer to this as IR or document it as such.

Across all sites, the most frequently observed aspect of IR was the completion of IR documentation. Overall, 86% of all IR interactions were observed to be documented. Seventy-five per cent of rounds were documented immediately after they were undertaken, whereas 9% of rounds were documented retrospectively and 2% were documented prospectively. Case study sites varied markedly in their approach towards documentation of IR. Documentation was completed most regularly at case study site 1, where 92% of rounds observed were documented, usually immediately after the round had taken place (85%). In comparison, 67% of rounds observed at case study site 3 were documented and 51% were completed immediately after the round. More rounds were completed retrospectively at case study site 3 (16%) than at sites 1 (6%) or 2 (8%), and rounds were also more likely to go undocumented in site 3 (30%) than at site 1 (8%) or site 2 (10%). The majority of rounds were documented at case study site 2 (88%), although only 77% were completed immediately after the round and prospective completion was noted more often here than at other sites (3%, compared with 1% at site 1 and 0% at site 3). This was because case study site 2 consisted primarily of single rooms, with IR documentation stored outside a patient’s room. Staff were therefore occasionally observed to complete the IR documentation before they entered the room to undertake the round.

The only other aspect of IR that was more commonly observed than not was the use of an opening phrase. Nursing staff used an opening phrase in 53% of the intentional rounds observed. However, it was rare for nursing staff to introduce themselves by name (as per the Studer Group protocol); this occurred in only 16% of interactions. Nursing staff tended to introduce themselves by name when meeting a patient for the first time, when the patient had new visitors with them or when a period of time had lapsed since they had last seen the patient (e.g. if the staff member had not been on shift for a couple of days). It was far more common (in 36% of interactions) for nursing staff to simply use a generic opening statement, such as ‘Morning’, ‘Hello’, ‘Are you allright?’ or ‘How are you feeling?’ when they entered the room or approached a patient’s bed. Occasionally, nursing staff opened the conversation with a direct question, such as did they want to get back into bed or had they opened their bowels, rather than begin with a greeting, per se. However, in 47% of rounds observed, no opening phrase was used by nursing staff at all – they simply approached the patient and started undertaking their tasks. Again, there were notable differences between sites in the use of opening phrases. Nursing staff at case study sites 2 and 3 were far more likely to use any form of opening statement than those at site 1, with opening phrases observed in 74% of IR interactions at site 3, 65% of IR interactions at site 2 and 27% of IR interactions at site 1.

Findings for all ‘4 P’s’ of IR were similar, in that these issues were rarely observed to be asked about by nursing staff. ‘Positioning’, ‘personal needs’, ‘pain’ and ‘placement of items’ questions were observed to be asked in 27%, 26%, 26% and 23% of rounds, respectively. This pattern of not asking about the ‘4 P’s’ was noted across all sites, although staff at case study site 1 were less likely to ask any of the ‘4 P’s’ questions than staff at the other sites. One other notable difference was that staff at case study site 3 were more likely to ask about personal needs (49%) than staff at sites 1 (13%) or 2 (27%). Environmental assessments were also uncommon, with these being observed in only 8% of all IR interactions. Again, staff at case study site 3 were observed to undertake environmental assessments more often (14%) than staff at sites 1 (8%) or 2 (6%).

A closing phrase, in any form, was observed in only 9% of all interactions. According to the Studer Group protocol, nursing staff are required to ask the question ‘Is there anything else I can do for you?’, but the staff in this study were observed asking this question (or words to that affect, e.g. ‘Do you need anything?’) in only 3% of rounds. Staff at case study site 2 were more likely (5%) to ask this question than staff at sites 1 (1%) or 2 (0%), although there may be some reasonable explanations for this finding (e.g. a larger number of single rooms). As shown in Table 12, staff at case study site 3 were not required to ask this question; therefore, it is unsurprising that no one did. Furthermore, case study site 2 was the only site where staff were required to sign the IR documentation to confirm that they had asked this question, whereas staff at site 1 were supposed to ask it but did not need to document it. This requirement to document the closing phrase on the form may explain why staff at site 2 were more likely to ask this question than those from other sites. However, it is questionable why only 5% of nursing staff were observed to ask this question, given their requirement to document this on the IR form. Using a generic closing statement to end rounds was observed slightly more often (in 6% of all rounds observed) than asking the Studer Group closing question. Statements such as ‘thanks’, ‘bye’ or ‘shout me if you want anything’ were used and others informed patients to use their call bell if they required any assistance. However, in 90% of rounds observed, nursing staff used no closing statement at all; they simply completed the round and left. Occasionally, this was because the nurse had been interrupted by another member of staff at this point and had terminated the round prematurely in order to provide assistance elsewhere. However, leaving without saying anything was less common in case study site 2 (83%) than in sites 1 (97%) or 3 (95%); this may be because of the predominance of single rooms in this site. It may be that it feels less comfortable for nursing staff to leave a single room without giving a closing statement than it does for those leaving a bedside in a shared bay. The end of an intentional round delivered in a single room may feel more finite to staff when they have to physically leave the room, rather than for those working in a bay setting, who may simply be returning to their bay-based desk or moving to the patient in the next bed.

Finally, Studer Group protocols state that nursing staff should inform the patient when they will next return to them, but this was observed in < 1% of all interactions (0.8%). It was slightly more common (3%) for staff to give a vague suggestion that they would be returning to the patient (e.g. ‘see you later’ or ‘see you in a bit’) but far more common (95%) for staff not to mention anything about coming back. This propensity for staff to not mention when they would be returning to the patient was observed across all sites, but was slightly less common at case study site 3 (88%) than at sites 1 (98%) or 2 (96%). It was also rare for RNs to be observed checking HCA’s IR documentation, as this was observed on only three occasions (once at site 1 and twice at site 2).

Overall, Table 20 demonstrates that IR was not being delivered in the English case study sites in the same manner promoted by the Studer Group. In these sites, the main focus of IR appeared to be in the completion of IR documentation, regardless of whether or not the IR questions had been asked. What is clear from these findings is that the successful completion of IR paperwork is not necessarily indicative of fundamental care being delivered by nursing staff during the rounds. At case study site 1, where the regular completion of IR documentation was most successful (i.e. completed for 92% of observed interactions), nursing staff were the least likely to ask patients about any aspect of the four P’s and least likely to open their interactions with any form of greeting or inform patients when they would return. Indeed, staff at this site were observed more than at any other to simply complete IR documentation without asking patients any of the questions on the form. Comparatively, at case study site 3, where the regular completion of IR documentation was least successful (i.e. completed for only 67% of interactions), nursing staff were the most likely to be observed undertaking a number of aspects of IR (e.g. using an opening phrase, asking about personal needs, undertaking an environmental assessment and informing patients when they would return) – they just did not always document to say that they had done so. These findings suggest that health-care managers should exercise some caution when relying on IR documentation for audit purposes or in response to incidents or complaints: the completion of IR documentation does not necessarily mean that fundamental care has been delivered and vice versa.

Intentional rounding adaptations

In England, IR implementers have adapted the version of IR that was originally promoted by the Studer Group in six different ways:

  1. reach
  2. frequency
  3. content
  4. style
  5. responsibility
  6. documentation.

Reach

In theory, if IR was conceptualised, broadly and simply, to mean checking regularly to ensure that patients were comfortable, safe, not in pain and had everything they needed, all patients would benefit from IR and this would mean that no one would be missed or neglected. Towards the end of this study, we learned that this was all that was originally intended by UK policy-makers when introducing IR in England. In practice, however, the Studer Group protocol for IR is more specific and less relevant for some patients and in some settings.

Frequency

The regularity of IR varied according to:

  • patient characteristics
  • patient ‘risk’ assessment
  • time of day/night.

Content

During English implementation, the Studer Group protocol content had been adapted in the following ways:

  • Irrelevant questions had been omitted (e.g. mobile/independent patients were not asked if they needed a change of position or to go to the toilet).
  • Patient-relevant questions/checks were added (e.g. vital signs, food–fluid charts, skin checks, wound care, i.v. lines and infusions, pressure ulcer risk assessment, incontinence check, nutrition check, falls prevention).
  • Response format had changed (e.g. Y = yes, N = no, UC = unable to communicate, NA = not applicable vs. tick for yes, cross for no, etc.).

Style

Intentional rounding could be carried out using a script and asking closed questions or by being incorporated into a general conversation and asking open questions. For a variety of reasons, some nurses undertook IR in a task-orientated, tick-box way, rather than using it as an opportunity to engage the patient in conversation to find out more about them and develop a rapport/relationship.

Responsibility

The responsibility for completing IR was shared between HCAs and RNs (and sometimes student nurses), but the proportion of IR that was done by each group varied. Furthermore, the strategies for ensuring that RNs took overall responsibility for IR differed (e.g. one site changed their documentation layout to ensure that RNs signed to say they had taken overall responsibility, even if some IR checks were completed by HCAs).

Documentation

Adaptations were made to IR documentation in a variety of ways:

  • Layout – whether staff had to enter the specific time the round was completed or if time slots of every 1 or 2 hours were already recorded on the form; certain components of the form were colour coded to ensure that they were completed by RNs, etc.
  • Clarity of instructions/guidance – for example where there was the inclusion of a body map to note pressure damage areas and the presence of devices.
  • Instructions on the regularity of specific aspects of IR adaption – where pressure ulcer checks were included in IR the frequency of these checks was dependent on patients’ assessed risk, for example ‘high risk’ (every 2 hours minimum), ‘medium risk’ (every 4 hours) or ‘low risk’ (daily).
  • Complexity – for example, with regard to documentation, ‘keys’ such as ‘position or links to other screening tools embedded in IR (e.g. Nutrition Malnutrition screening tool).
  • IR follow-up identified and documented – whether or not there was space for nursing staff to record any variation/deviance or any actions resulting from rounding (e.g. pain control, medication administered).
  • Ease of access – whether or not IR documentation was kept at end of patients’ beds, outside their room or at the nursing station.
  • Where IR is recorded – for example, was it recorded on a separate form or in a nursing document bundle of other assessments and records, etc.?
  • Form – whether IR documentation was kept in an electronic or paper format.

Detailed revised contexts–mechanisms–outcomes

A detailed exploration of the refined CMOs that informed the revised programme theory are provided in Table 21.

TABLE 21

TABLE 21

Summary of eight revised CMO configurations

Using theories to develop contexts–mechanisms–outcomes

The national survey of senior nursing managers, interviews (with front-line nurses, senior nursing managers, other health-care staff, patients and carers), observation of patients (to complete Qualpacs) and shadowing of individual nurses all provided data to support and refute the CMOs. The findings of this study support other work that suggests that some mechanisms operate on a continuum, rather than simply the mechanism ‘firing’ or ‘not firing’.72 In this section, the existing literature and theories are drawn on to develop two of the eight mechanisms that we have concluded were partially activated, namely CMO 1, ‘consistency and comprehensiveness’, and CMO 3, ‘accountability’.

Context–mechanism–outcome 1: consistency and comprehensiveness

Is intentional rounding a task-orientated or worker-orientated activity?

One of the tensions underpinning IR is the degree to which it should be structured, standardised and applied to everyone in a consistent and comprehensive way or used in a selective and flexible way. When analysing jobs, occupational psychologists make the distinction between jobs or roles that are primarily task-orientated activities (i.e. anyone can do it if they have a checklist) and worker-orientated activities (i.e. requires knowledge and skill that goes beyond any checklist).73

In its original US form, IR falls nearer the task-orientated end of the continuum. This study has revealed the huge variation in the way IR has been implemented in England and the many site-specific adaptations that have extended the remit of IR beyond the original US form. These site-specific adaptations include additional comfort and safety checks (e.g. vital signs, food and fluid charts, wound care, i.v. lines and infusions, pressure ulcer risk assessment, falls prevention) and the using of IR selectively. Arguably, these additional comfort and safety checks and the tailoring of IR to particular patients, settings and circumstances require additional nursing knowledge, skill and professional judgement, meaning that IR then becomes more of a ‘worker-orientated activity’. This has implications for practice, in terms how the work is allocated and supervised and what additional knowledge, skill and professional judgement is required to conduct IR.

Use of checklists to improve safety

Checklists have been used to improve safety in the aviation industry and imported into health care, for example in the World Health Organization surgical safety checklist.74 IR would be an example of a checklist that is used for normal everyday procedures. Normal everyday checklists are effective whenever there are:

. . . advantages to standardising performance, time is not critical, the series of tasks is too long to be committed to memory (or there are likely to be interruptions to execution of the task that might interfere with memory retrieval), and the environment enables a physical list to be accessed and used.

Clay-Williams and Colligan75

Intentional rounding checklists differ from those used in aviation in a number of ways, outlined in the following sections.

Standardising performance

The nursing environment requires an approach with more flexibility than that found on the flight deck. Although every flight is different, there is more variation in hospitals (e.g. types of patients, settings, ward layouts) than on the flight deck. When the IR tasks become more complex, the role of nursing staff in completing and/or supervising IR tasks and completing documentation needs to be clear.

Time

This study has shown that nurses are encouraged to conduct IR around other tasks, rather than as a discreet activity. Although IR is not a time-critical activity, it is conducted regularly on an hourly basis or every 2 hours.

Series of tasks

The IR checklist in its original US form is not long (e.g. the 4Ps, an opening and closing phrase, an environmental scan and telling the patient when the nurse will return).

Ongoing versus one-off

Unlike aviation checklists, which are designed for one-off specific occasions (pre flight, after take-off, before landing), IR checklists are used repeatedly throughout the day and night.

Context–mechanism–outcome 3: accountability

Another tension underpinning IR is its contribution to supporting accountability for nursing care. This exists at several levels, including from the point of care to senior management and commissioning.

Accountability for own actions, covered by the Nursing and Midwifery Council code (2018)

There are numerous definitions of individual accountability, but, essentially, it is each nurse accepting responsibility for their nursing practice. RNs, as well as being legally accountable, are accountable to several stakeholders: their employer, regulator [the Nursing and Midwifery Council (NMC)] and the person receiving care, whose requirements may at times conflict.

Delegating authority, covered by the Nursing and Midwifery Council code

The NMC also requires a RN to retain accountability for nursing tasks delegated to other staff.76 Delegation was not specifically mentioned in the Francis report,1 although staffing policies of reducing the skill mix of registered to unregistered staff were considered to have contributed to declining professionalism and tolerance of poorer standards of care. The NMC code77 requires that nurses delegate tasks that are within the competence of the other person, to supervise and support them so that they can provide safe and compassionate care and to confirm that the completed task has been delivered to the required standard. This is a challenge, particularly when wards are short staffed and the team includes temporary staff. Little is known about how care is delegated, which is usually locally determined,78 although it can cause anxiety for newly qualified RNs.79 This study has demonstrated that IR is predominantly valued by nurses as a way of providing this evidence for care delivered by themselves and, to a larger degree, that they have delegated to an unregistered nursing assistant. It is argued that the risk-averse and blame culture in the nursing and health-care environment leads to RNs needing evidence that the care they are accountable for providing has been delivered and IR does provide them with this reassurance.

Evaluating performance, a trust requirement

Evidence in this study suggests that IR documentation is being used to demonstrate that care has been delivered when performance is being challenged, for example complaints, inquests and untoward incidents. However, this study has demonstrated that documentation, for various reasons, does not reflect the care that was delivered at the time. Furthermore, senior nurses were aware of this and did not rely on IR alone but reviewed the whole patient record.

One case study trust used the IR documentation as a performance measurement in their service development programme. This raises the question of whether or not IR is a ‘good’ or ‘adequate’ measure of performance. In a review of metrics for nursing, indicators were seen to provide information about care delivery and how it can be improved, and to monitor performance against agreed standards.80 They advised against the use of measures that focus on care processes rather than outcome, as they were most vulnerable to what they called gaming and/or perverse incentives. This study has shown that the standard to which IR should be delivered is ambiguous, with nurses and senior nurses valuing flexibility in conducting IR, contrary, in some cases, to explicit hospital policy. Furthermore, IR is vulnerable to intentional and unintentional gaming; thus, it is argued that IR is a poor measure of nursing performance. Approaches to evaluating the effectiveness of performance should involve proper evaluation techniques that are capable of measuring performance through the use of clearly defined, achievable and measurable goals that set out what ‘good performance’ looks like. Currently, it is not surprising that nurses have concluded that ‘good performance’ in relation to IR means completing IR documentation, as this is what is audited by senior nursing staff.

Summary

  • In the revised, evidence-informed programme theory, only two of the original eight mechanisms were partially activated (‘consistency and comprehensiveness’ and ‘accountability’). The evidence for two mechanisms was inconclusive (‘visibility of nurses’ and ‘anticipation’). There was minimal evidence for one mechanism (multidisciplinary teamwork and communication) and no evidence for the remaining three (‘allocated time to care’, ‘nurse–patient relationships and communication’ and ‘patient empowerment’).
  • The four layers of contextual factors that enabled or inhibited the activation of mechanisms were explored. These included type of patients; patient and carer awareness, understanding and involvement; nursing staff characteristics; leadership characteristics; implementation factors, such as staged or simultaneous implementation; staff engagement and motivation; staff education, training and understanding of IR; design and suitability of IR documentation; and environmental and structural factors, such as ward setting/layout, job demands and staffing levels, skill mix/workforce stability, senior nursing management/IR organisational policies, NHS context health policy and NHS culture.
  • A total of 240 IR interactions were observed over the course of 188 hours of care delivery observation. Although 86% of all IR interactions were observed to be documented, fidelity to the original intervention (i.e. the Studer Group protocol) was generally low.
Copyright © Queen’s Printer and Controller of HMSO 2019. This work was produced by Harris et al. under the terms of a commissioning contract issued by the Secretary of State for Health and Social Care. This issue may be freely reproduced for the purposes of private research and study and extracts (or indeed, the full report) may be included in professional journals provided that suitable acknowledgement is made and the reproduction is not associated with any form of advertising. Applications for commercial reproduction should be addressed to: NIHR Journals Library, National Institute for Health Research, Evaluation, Trials and Studies Coordinating Centre, Alpha House, University of Southampton Science Park, Southampton SO16 7NS, UK.
Bookshelf ID: NBK547462

Views

  • PubReader
  • Print View
  • Cite this Page
  • PDF version of this title (6.1M)

Other titles in this collection

Recent Activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...